Computational Theories of Mindfulness

From Psyc 40 Wiki
Jump to: navigation, search

By Liza Tatishev

Mindfulness involves non-judgmental moment-to-moment self-awareness of physical and mental states– it is the ability to be fully conscious and aware. Within the past decade, scientific interest in mindfulness practices has increased. Mindfulness has a long history: originating over 2,500 years ago in Indian Buddhist tradition [5]. By the mid-twentieth century, mindfulness became secularized and incorporated into psychological intervention methods to improve emotional well-being [6]. Today, mindfulness-based intervention methods are recognized as successful approaches in treating a variety of psychological disorders, including depression, anxiety, and addiction, thus making it clinically significant.

A more precise understanding of mindfulness can benefit the refinement of mindfulness-based intervention methods. The field of computational neuroscience offers a method to examine the neuroscience of mindfulness. In order to approach the neuroscience of mindfulness computationally, it is important to first clarify the physical reality of the brain that is being represented via algorithms.

Psychological Mechanisms

As a psychological intervention method, mindfulness practices– such as Tibetan Buddhist imagery meditation [7], Zen meditation [8], and Kundalini yoga [9]– are grounded in training and refining attention skills. Attentional control is a key cognitive process of executive functioning (also referred to as cognitive control) necessary for emotional and behavioral regulation [10].

Cognitive control

Mindfulness training has been shown to enhance cognitive control in children and adults [11]. On a phenomenological level, the process of mindfulness training engages with the self-regulation of cognitive states that are interrelated with attentional networks.

Within cognitive psychology, attention consists of sub-processes (including alerting, sustained attention, and conflict monitoring) that underlie the ability to attend to stimuli. Higher self-reported mindfulness is related to higher measures of more stable sustained attention [12]. Furthermore, longitudinal studies have found that engaging in mindfulness practices correlated with improvement in sustained attention tasks over the course of intervention [13].

Sustained attention recruits attentional control functions, while inhibiting proponent, but non-relevant responses [14]. Research has demonstrated that mindfulness may improve performance on attentional control tasks, however experimental designs should be improved to remove discrepancies in the field [15].

Evidence for mindfulness practice improving sustained attention and attentional control processes exists. Enhanced cognitive control network activity is associated with increased psychological well-being [16] [17] [18].

Therapies

Mindfulness offers a framework of self-awareness, -regulation, and -transcendence that can be implemented in psychological intervention methods [19]. Mindfulness is now incorporated as a major component of empirically supported psychological intervention methods, including mindfulness-based stress reduction (MSBR) [20], mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) [21], acceptance and commitment therapy, and dialectical behavior therapy [22].

Figure 1. The Liverpool Mindfulness Model. This model relates motivational factors to core mental processes and behavioral outcomes [1]

Neurophysiological mechanisms

The cognitive and behavioral effects of mindfulness can be examined through the theoretical framework of emotion regulation. Studies on the neuroscience of mindfulness demonstrate that individuals who engage in mindfulness demonstrate functional and structural differences in the emotional regulation network– specifically, by strengthening the ability to regulate ruminative thinking [23]. Effective emotional regulation facilitates psychological well-being [24].

Brain activity

Early electroencephalogram (EEG) studies and behavioral studies have demonstrated that mindfulness practices engage particular attentional networks. Mindfulness practices produce state changes that increase activation of core nodes in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) of the central executive network (CEN) (areas associated with executive function and attention) [25]. Other associated neural networks that show increased activity with mindfulness practices include the anterior subdivision of the cingulate cortex (ACC) [26] and the insular cortex (IC) [27].

Figure 2. Attention regulation during meditation. Panel A presents a schematic representation of the meditation process, wherein the inner circle outlines the phenomenological process, the middle circle relates the inner circle with attentional processes, and the outer circle relates attentional processes with brain networks. Panel B presents the main brain regions involved in each network of the outer circle in Panel A. [2]

Mindfulness training can result in the upregulation of underlying neural networks in brain activity at a resting state [28], which is correlated with significant improvement in emotion regulation [29]. Causal evidence for neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the cognitive benefits of mindfulness training exist. For example, children who receive mindfulness training intervention exhibit a stronger preserved resting state default mode network (DMN)-DLPFC anticorrelation (decreased activation of the DMN and increased activation in the CEN) compared to controls [30]. Enhanced DMN-DLPFC anticorrelation correlates with better sustained attention performance [31].

Computational theories

Attentional control and emotion regulation are prime targets for computational modeling. However, comprehensive computational frameworks on the effect mindfulness practises have on emotion and cognition are nascent [32].

Bayesian brain hypothesis

One potential computational mechanism that has been proposed to understand the cognitive processes of MBCT is the “Bayesian brain” hypothesis which suggests that the brain creates and continuously update a model of the external world (the physical environment and the physical self) based on input of past experience and homeostatic needs [33]. Neurophysiological studies have begun testing mechanisms proposed by the Bayesian brain hypothesis. For example, EEG studies have demonstrated that mathematical predictions of processes by Bayesian brain theories coincide with brain activity [34] [35] [36]. Predictive coding suggests how cognitive mechanisms are implemented physiologically, which allows for concrete and experimentally testable predictions for the neurophysiological processes affected by MBCT [37]. For example, changes in attentional focus change the precision weighting of sensory channels that are being attended to [38]. Thus, theories in predictive coding coincide with the training phase of MBCT, wherein the practitioner learns to maintain and re-orient attentional focus [39].

Figure 3. Panel A shows a graphical summary of Bayes' theory on which the Bayesian brain hypothesis is established on. Illustrates that posterior beliefs depends on the distribution of prior belief and likelihood, wherein PE is the abbreviation for "prediction error." Panel B shows that when the precision of the prior belief is higher than the precision of likelihood, the update in the posterior belief stays close to the prior belief (there is a small update). Panel C shows that when the precision of likelihood is higher than the precision of the prior belief, the update in the posterior belief moves strongly toward the data (there is a large update) [3]
Figure 4. Graphical summary of predictive coding. This panel represents the case when an individual adopts "the being mode" of MCBT (to cultivate acceptance). Before adopting this cognitive method, perception is strongly shaped by priors, whereas after attentional modulation may lead to rapid belief updates, shifting the posterior belief towards likelihood [4]

Processes within meditation training, such as MBCT training, can be understood from the perspective of Bayesian models of brain function by deriving experimentally testable predictions between attentional focus and sensory precision. Therefore, computational modeling becomes a clinical tool to obtain subject-specific estimates of precision-weighted prediction errors about physiological states.

Activation propagation model

The activation propagation model aims to explain the information propagation mechanism [40] of the brain (cognitive activity arises from large-scale brain network structure) by modeling neural activity at a macroscopic level, wherein the activity of each brain region at a given time is updated by the amount of input the region receives [41].

The activation propagation model of the brain can be used to simulate how mindfulness practices affect information propagation in the neural network structure of certain brain regions. Neural activity in brain regions can be related to postulates of emotion regulation and behavior.

References

Siegel, R.D., Germer, C.K., Olendzki, A. (2009). Mindfulness: What Is It? Where Did It Come From?. In: Didonna, F. (eds) Clinical Handbook of Mindfulness. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-09593-6_2

Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. (2010). The effect of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(2), 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018555

Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., & d'Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry research, 106(2), 113–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0925-4927(01)00074-9

Ritskes, R., Ritskes-Hoitinga, M., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Bærentsen, K., & Hartman, T. (2003). MRI Scanning During Zen Meditation: The Picture of Enlightenment? Constructivism in the Human Sciences, 8(1), 85–90.

Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, G. L., Khalsa, G., & Benson, H. (2000). Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. Neuroreport, 11(7), 1581–1585. Malinowski P. (2013). Neural mechanisms of attentional control in mindfulness meditation. Frontiers in neuroscience, 7, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00008

Cásedas, L., Pirruccio, V., Vadillo, M.A. et al. Does Mindfulness Meditation Training Enhance Executive Control? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials in Adults. Mindfulness 11, 411–424 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01279-4 Treadway, M. T., & Lazar, S. W. (2009). The neurobiology of mindfulness. In F. Didonna (Ed.), Clinical handbook of mindfulness (pp. 45–57). Springer Science + Business Media. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-09593-6_4

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "Frontal Lobe" tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Yang, Z., Oathes, D. J., Linn, K. A., Bruce, S. E., Satterthwaite, T. D., Cook, P. A., Satchell, E. K., Shou, H., & Sheline, Y. I. (2018). Cognitive Behavioral Therapy Is Associated With Enhanced Cognitive Control Network Activity in Major Depression and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. Biological psychiatry. Cognitive neuroscience and neuroimaging, 3(4), 311–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.006

Quaglia, J. T., Zeidan, F., Grossenbacher, P. G., Freeman, S. P., Braun, S. E., Martelli, A., Goodman, R. J., & Brown, K. W. (2019). Brief mindfulness training enhances cognitive control in socioemotional contexts: Behavioral and neural evidence. PloS one, 14(7), e0219862. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219862

Kayser, J., Wong, L.Y.X., Sacchi, E. et al. Behavioral measures of attention and cognitive control during a new auditory working memory paradigm. Behav Res 52, 1161–1174 (2020). https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01308-z Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 6, 296. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296

Kabat-Zinn J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General hospital psychiatry, 4(1), 33–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3

Elices, M., Pérez-Sola, V., Pérez-Aranda, A., Colom, F., Polo, M., Martín-López, L. M., & Gárriz, M. (2022). The Effectiveness of Mindfulness-Based Cognitive Therapy in Primary Care and the Role of Depression Severity and Treatment Attendance. Mindfulness, 13(2), 362–372. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-021-01794-3

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33–47. doi:10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3.

A. J. Moye and M. K. van Vugt, "A Computational Model of Focused Attention Meditation and Its Transfer to a Sustained Attention Task," in IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, vol. 12, no. 2, pp. 329-339, 1 April-June 2021, doi: 10.1109/TAFFC.2019.2908172.

Paus T. (2001). Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 2(6), 417–424. https://doi.org/10.1038/35077500

Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(27), 11483–11488. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0606552104

Zhang, Z., Luh, WM., Duan, W. et al. Longitudinal effects of meditation on brain resting-state functional connectivity. Sci Rep 11, 11361 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90729-y Ng, H. H., Wu, C. W., Huang, F. Y., Cheng, Y. T., Guu, S. F., Huang, C. M., Hsu, C. F., Chao, Y. P., Jung, T. P., & Chuang, C. H. (2021). Mindfulness Training Associated With Resting-State Electroencephalograms Dynamics in Novice Practitioners via Mindful Breathing and Body-Scan. Frontiers in psychology, 12, 748584. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.748584

Bauer, C. C. C., Rozenkrantz, L., Caballero, C., Nieto-Castanon, A., Scherer, E., West, M. R., Mrazek, M., Phillips, D. T., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2020). Mindfulness training preserves sustained attention and resting state anticorrelation between default-mode network and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: A randomized controlled trial. Human brain mapping, 41(18), 5356–5369. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25197

Denkova, E., Nomi, J. S., Uddin, L. Q., & Jha, A. P. (2019). Dynamic brain network configurations during rest and an attention task with frequent occurrence of mind wandering. Human brain mapping, 40(15), 4564–4576. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24721

Manjaly, Z. M., & Iglesias, S. (2020). A Computational Theory of Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy from the "Bayesian Brain" Perspective. Frontiers in psychiatry, 11, 404. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00404

Gordon, N., Koenig-Robert, R., Tsuchiya, N., van Boxtel, J. J., & Hohwy, J. (2017). Neural markers of predictive coding under perceptual uncertainty revealed with Hierarchical Frequency Tagging. eLife, 6, e22749. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.22749

Iglesias, S., Mathys, C., Brodersen, K. H., Kasper, L., Piccirelli, M., den Ouden, H. E., & Stephan, K. E. (2013). Hierarchical prediction errors in midbrain and basal forebrain during sensory learning. Neuron, 80(2), 519–530. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.009

Feldman, H., & Friston, K. J. (2010). Attention, uncertainty, and free-energy. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 4, 215. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2010.00215

Nakamura, H., Tawatsuji, Y., Fang, S., & Matsui, T. (2021). Explanation of emotion regulation mechanism of mindfulness using a brain function model. Neural networks : the official journal of the International Neural Network Society, 138, 198–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2021.01.029